Is Race A Valid Taxonomic Construct?
Internet Essay: 14 December 2001


J. Philippe Rushton
Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario

[This internet essay is Rushton’s latest “Statement on Race.” It updates the research in his book Race,
Evolution, and Behavior (3rd edition, 2000) which contains over 1,000 references to the literature.]

The answer to the question posed in the title of this paper is “yes.” If the concept of race
didn’t exist, science would have to invent it (and did)! Race is a valid taxonomic construct
because it allows us to make predictions about people’s behavior, especially at the group level.

In science, a concept is useful if it groups facts so that general laws and conclusions can
be drawn from them. Predictions can be made using the taxonomic category of race because, on
average, the Chinese, Japanese, and Koreans are similar to each other and different from White
Americans, Germans, and Russians, who are similar to each other and different from Black
Americans, Haitians, and sub-Saharan Africans. Predictability is the criterion by which the value
of a hypothetical construct like race is evaluated. As I will show, race is highly predictive.

For the past 20 years my research has focused on differences between the three major
races, commonly termed Orientals (East Asians, Mongoloids), Whites (Europeans, Caucasoids),
and Blacks (Africans, Negroids). Roughly speaking, Orientals are those who have most of their
ancestors from East Asia. Whites have most of their ancestors from Europe. And Blacks have
most of their ancestors from sub-Saharan Africa. In the main, I have not addressed the many
other groups outside of these three major races, or sub-groups within the three major races,
though they are of interest as well.

What I've found is that in brain size, intelligence, temperament, sexual behavior, fertility,
growth rate, life span, crime, and family stability, Orientals, as a group, consistently fall at one
end of the spectrum, Blacks fall at the other end, and Whites fall in between. On average,
Orientals are slower to mature, less fertile, and less sexually active, and have larger brains and
higher IQ scores. Blacks are at the opposite end in each of these areas. Whites fall in the middle,
often close to Orientals (see Chart 1).




Of course, these three-way racial differences are averages. Individuals are individuals.
However, I've found that this three-way pattern is consistently true over time and across nations.
That the same three-way racial pattern occurs repeatedly on some 60 different biological and
behavioral variables is profoundly interesting and shows that race is more than “just skin deep.”
The international data come from the World Health Organization, the United Nations, and
Interpol. Recently, I even traveled to South Africa to collect new IQ data.

Let's start with the biological differences in sports, which is something almost everyone
observes. Jon Entine's recent book Taboo: Why Black Athletes Dominate Sports and Why We
Are Afraid to Talk About It, addresses the old cliché that "White men can't jump" (and the new
one that Oriental men jump even less well). Entine shows that in sports, it is Black men and
women who can sky! And yet, as the data also show, it is mainly Blacks of West African descent
who excel at running over short distances, while Blacks of East African descent –from Kenya
and Ethiopia – excel at marathon running over long distances. These differences between East
and West Africans show that taking an average can sometimes gloss over important distinctions.
Still, Blacks from both East and West Africa excel at one or another kind of running. In sports,
Blacks as a group, have a genetic advantage.

It is interesting to know that race differences show up early in life. Black babies are born
a week earlier than White babies, yet they mature faster as measured by bone development. By
age five or six, Black children excel in the dash, the long jump, and the high jump, all of which
require a short burst of power. By the teenage years, Blacks have faster reflexes, as in the famous
knee-jerk response.

Blacks also have from 3 to 19% more of the sex hormone testosterone than Whites or
Orientals. This means more explosive energy, which gives Blacks the edge in sports like boxing,
basketball, football, and sprinting.

Why is it taboo to say that Blacks are on average better at sports? Because the hormones
that give Blacks the edge in sports also make them more masculine in general. They are
physically more active in school, and this can sometimes get them into trouble or even lead to
their being diagnosed as hyperactive.

So the next question is, “Why do East Asians and Whites and have wider hips than
Blacks, and so make poorer runners?” The answer is that they give birth to larger brained babies.
During evolution, as the head size of newborns increased, women had to have a wider pelvis.
Orientals average 1 cubic inch more cranial capacity than Whites, and Whites average a very
large 5 cubic inches more cranial capacity than Blacks.

Some people are surprised to hear that the races differ in brain size. And they wonder
how convincing the evidence is that brain size is related to intelligence. In fact, dozens of studies,
including those based on state-of-the-art magnetic resonance imaging, have demonstrated the
relation between brain size and intelligence.

Race differences in brain size have been demonstrated using four different methods: (1)
magnetic resonance imaging, (2) brain weight at autopsy, (3) endocranial volume, and (4)
external head measurements. These data are summarized in Chart 2 which presents the brain size
averages across the four measurement techniques and also, where possible, corrected for body
size. Orientals averaged 1,364 cm3, Whites averaged 1,347 cm3, and Blacks averaged 1,267 cm3.

Naturally the averages vary between samples and the races do overlap. But the results from
different methods on different samples, measured from the 1840s to the 1990s, show the same
strong pattern.





The racial differences in brain size show up at birth. One study of my own, published in
the 1997 issue of the journal Intelligence, was carried out using the resources of the National
Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke (NINCDS) in Bethesda,
Maryland. In it, I analyzed data from the enormous Collaborative Perinatal Project, which took
head circumference measures and IQ scores from over 50,000 children followed from birth to
seven years. The Oriental children averaged larger head circumferences than did the White
children at birth, four months, one year, and seven years; the White children averaged larger head
circumferences than did the Black children (see Chart 3).




I published several other studies during the 1990s, also in Intelligence, confirming the
racial differences in brain size. In one study, I (1991) analyzed data from the National
Aeronautics and Space Administration, and found the mean cranial capacity for East Asians was
1,460 cm3, and for Europeans it was 1,446 cm3. From a stratified random sample of 6,325 U.S.
Army personnel, I (1992) calculated average cranial capacities for Asians, Whites, and Blacks,
respectively, of 1,416 cm3, 1,380 cm3, and 1,359 cm3. (I also found that officers averaged 1,393
cm3 while enlisted personnel averaged 1,375 cm3.) From a compilation made by the International
Labour Office in Geneva of tens of thousands of people from around the world, I (1994) found
that samples from the Pacific Rim, Europe, and Africa averaged cranial capacities, respectively,
of 1,308 cm3, 1,297 cm3, and 1,241 cm3. Travis Osborne and I (1995) published a paper showing
that brain size was about 50% heritable for both Blacks and Whites using data from the Georgia
Twin Study based on 236 pairs of Black and White adolescent twins. And once again we found
Whites averaged greater cranial capacity than Blacks.

Since one cubic inch of brain matter contains millions of brain cells and hundreds of
millions of nerve connections, brain size helps to explain why the races differ in IQ. On
standardized IQ tests, hundreds of studies show the three-way pattern. Orientals average slightly
ahead of Whites on such tests and Whites average substantially ahead of Blacks. Most IQ tests
have an average score of 100, with a “normal” range from 85 to 115. Around the world, Whites
average an IQ of about 100, Orientals an IQ of about 104, and Blacks in Britain, the Caribbean,
and the U.S. average lower IQs -- about 85. The lowest average IQs are found for sub-Saharan
Africans – around 70 (see Chart 4). Like the other data sets in this essay, these are reviewed in
my book Race, Evolution, and Behavior. An even more recent book on the topic is by Richard
Lynn and Tatu Vanhanen, IQ and the Wealth of Nations.



The extremely low average IQ of 70 for sub-Saharan Africans has been difficult for many
people to accept. To determine for myself how realistic a figure it was, I traveled to South Africa
to initiate a series of studies at the University of the Witwatersrand in Johannesburg, one of the
most prestigious universities in Africa. In the first study, we administered the Raven’s Standard
Progressive Matrices to 173 African first year psychology students who scored at the 14th
percentile on 1993 U.S. norms, giving them an average IQ of 84. In a second study, we
administered the same test to another group of psychology students who scored an IQ of 83.
After training on how to solve these types of tests, their IQs rose to 96. In a third study, we gave
the same test to a more academically select student population -- 198 African engineering
students who had taken mathematics and sciences courses in high school. This group scored at
the 41st percentile with an IQ of 97. These results, from an elite university, showing mean IQ
scores for African undergraduates ranging from 83 to 97, confirm the overall IQ of 70 for
Africans because around the world university students typically score 15 to 30 IQ points above
their population average.

Race differences in brain size and IQ, along with those in testosterone, have important
implications for social behavior. For example, in the United States, Orientals are seen as a
“model minority.” They have fewer divorces, out-of-wedlock births, and cases of child abuse
than do Whites. More Orientals graduate from college and fewer go to prison. Blacks, on the
other hand, are 12% of the American population but make up 50% of the prison population.

The racial pattern of crime in the U.S. is not due to local conditions like “White racism.”
For nearly 20 years I have been monitoring the Interpol Yearbooks and publishing data on the
worldwide crime statistics. These consistently show that the rate of violent crime (murder, rape,
and serious assault) is about three times lower in East Asian and Pacific Rim countries than they
are in African and Caribbean countries. Whites in European countries are intermediate. The 1996
rates of violent crime, for example, were: East Asian countries, 35 per 100,000 people; European
countries, 42; and African and Caribbean countries, 149 (see Chart 5).




Orientals are the least sexually active, whether measured by age of first intercourse,
intercourse frequency, or number of sexual partners. Blacks are the most active on all of these.
Once again Whites fall in between. National surveys in Britain and the U.S., and international
surveys by the World Health Organization, reveal the three-way racial pattern in sexual behavior.
These racial differences, in turn, affect the rate of sexually transmitted diseases. For example, the
latest figures from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (for the year 2000) shows the
rate of chlamydia in African Americans is 10 times higher than it is for Whites, and for
gonorrhea and syphilis the rate among Blacks is nearly 30 times the White rate.

Sadly, racial differences are also reflected in the current AIDS crisis. Over 40 million
people around the world are living with HIV/AIDS. Chart 6 shows the HIV infection rates in
various parts of the world based on the latest figures from UNAIDS (December 2001). The
epidemic started in Black Africa in the late 1970s. Today 28 million adults there are living with
HIV/AIDS. Over fifty percent of these are female. This shows that transmission is mainly
heterosexual. Currently, nearly 9 out of every 100 Africans are infected with the AIDS virus and
the epidemic is considered out of control. In several countries the AIDS rate is over 20%,
including South Africa, where one in 5 adults is living with HIV/AIDS.



The HIV infection rate is also high in the Black Caribbean – 2.2%! The high rate of
HIV/AIDS in the 2,000-mile band of Caribbean countries extends from Bermuda to Guyana, and
it is highest in the Bahamas and in Haiti, where the rates are 4% and 5%, respectively.

Data published by the U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention show that African
Americans have HIV/AIDS rates similar to the Black Caribbean and parts of Black Africa. Three
percent of Black men and 1% of Black women in the U.S. are living with HIV. The rate for
White Americans is about 0.4%, while the rate for Asian Americans is less than 0.05%. Rates for
Europe and the Pacific Rim are also low. Of course AIDS is a serious public health problem for
all racial groups, but it is especially so for Africans and people of African descent.

Importantly, the races also differ in rate of ovulation. Not all women produce just one egg
during the menstrual cycle. When two or more eggs are produced at the same time, pregnancy
and the likelihood of producing two-egg twins are more likely. The number of such twins born is
16 out of every 1,000 births for Blacks, 8 out of every 1,000 births for Whites, and 4 or less for
Orientals. Black women also average shorter menstrual cycles than White women. These and
other data make it plain that the race differences in reproductive behavior are biological in nature.

Twin and adoption studies show that genes play a big part in athletic ability, brain size,
IQ, and personality. Trans-racial adoption studies, where infants of one race are adopted and
reared by parents of a different race, provide some of the strongest evidence that race differences
are heritable. Oriental children, even if malnourished before being adopted by White parents, go
on to have IQs above the White average. Black infants adopted into middle-class White families
end up with IQs lower than the White average. Some of these data are summarized in Chart 7.



The Evolution of Racial Differences

Look back again at all the traits in Chart 1. They form a pattern. Whites consistently
average between Orientals and Blacks in dozens of areas. Also, the groups with the largest brains
have the lowest rates of two-egg twinning! Why? The answer lies in evolution. No purely
cultural theory can explain all of these traits taken together. There is, however, a gene-based
explanation that explicitly involves the trade-off between reproductive effort (twinning rates) and
brain size. The patterns make up what is called a “life-history.” They evolved together to meet
the trials of life -- survival, growth, and reproduction.

I have explained the racial pattern in brain size, intelligence, and other traits using a gene-
based life-history theory that evolutionary biologists call the r-K scale of reproductive
strategies. At one end of this scale are r-strategies that rely on high reproductive rates. At the
other end are K-strategies that rely on high levels of parental care. This scale is generally used to
compare the life histories of different species of animals. I have used it to explain the smaller but
real differences between the human races.

On this scale, Orientals are more K-selected than Whites, while Whites are more K-selected
than Blacks. Highly K-selected women produce fewer eggs (and have bigger brains) than r-selected
women. Highly K-selected men invest time and energy in their children rather than the pursuit of
sexual thrills. They are “dads” rather than “cads.”

Race differences also make sense in terms of human evolution. Modern humans evolved
in Africa about 200,000 years ago. Africans and non-Africans then split about 110,000 years ago.
Orientals and Whites split about 40,000 years ago, around the time that modern humans were
first in Europe. Analyses of DNA sequencing, along with the fossil and archaeological record,
demonstrate this sequence, as does the pattern of traits shown in Chart 1.

The further north people went out of Africa, the harder it was to get food, gain shelter,
make clothes, and raise children. So the groups that evolved into today’s Whites and Orientals
needed larger brains, more family stability, and a longer life. But building a bigger brain takes
time and energy so there is a trade off with slower rates of growth, lower levels of sex hormones,
less aggression, and less sexual activity. Thus came about the pattern of traits in Chart 1.

What are the implications of this research? One is, obviously, that race is a valid
taxonomic construct. If it were not it would have no reliable predictive value and we would not
find the same racial pattern all around the world and over time. The fact that, on average,

African-descended children are born with smaller brains than European- or East Asian-descended
children, regardless of where in the diasporas the children are located, allows reliable predictions
to be made about their future academic and occupational achievement. Similarly, the fact that
throughout the world Blacks have a stronger sex drive than Whites or East Asians explains why
Black Americans, Black Caribbeans, and sub-Saharan Africans have the highest rates of sexually
transmitted diseases, and why East Asians have even lower rates than Whites.

A second implication is that “White racism” is not responsible for all of society’s
problems. Black underachievement is not simply due to “White prejudice.” It is more deeply
rooted. On average, Black children are born with smaller brains than White or East Asian
children. Pointing this out is not constructing stereotypes, it is simply observing facts as they are.
Both science and justice call for us to seek and tell the truth, not to tell lies and spread error.

Another implication is that we have to accept that racial differences will not just
disappear. Hitherto, most theories in the behavioral sciences have assumed that all human
populations have equal abilities to achieve equal levels of social development. We need to accept
the existence of the evolved diversity of human populations.

Sometimes it is claimed by those who argue that race is just a social construct that the
human genome project shows that because people share 99% of their “genes” in common, that
there are no races. This is silly. Human genes are 98% similar to chimpanzee genes. Yet no one
thinks that chimpanzees have the same intelligence, brain size, or social behavior patterns as
human beings; they look and behave very differently. In fact humans share 90% of their genes
with mice, which is why we can use them to test drug therapies. Similarly, although men and
women are genetically 99% the same, it is foolish to believe that sex is just a “social
construction.”

Much confusion arises because there are several sets of genetic measures. A much more
realistic story comes from looking at the 3.1 billion base pairs that make up the 30,000 genes.
People differ in 1 out of every 1,000 of these base pairs. Each change in a base pair can alter a
gene. Technically, base pair differences are called single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs). Base
pair differences are important and SNPs clump together in races. Just one change in the base pair
for hemoglobin, for example, causes sickle-cell anemia, from which many Blacks suffer. Other
base pair differences affect IQ, aggression, and mental illness. The 3.1 billion base pairs provide
plenty of room for large racial differences.

In summary, the same racial pattern would not occur so consistently all around the world
and over time if race were a mere social construct. If it were a meaningless construct, it would
have no power to predict phenomena like brain size, growth rate, life span, crime, and family
stability. Other evidence also shows that race is a biological reality. For example, coroners in
crime labs can identify race from a skeleton or even just the skull. They can even identify race
from blood, hair, or semen. How could they do this if race was only a social construct? The
scientific evidence shows that the politically correct mantra “race is just skin deep” is a case of
deep denial.

Additional Readings

Beals, K. L., Smith, C. L., & Dodd, S. M. (1984). Brain size, cranial morphology, climate, and
time machines. Current Anthropology, 25, 301-330.
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., Menozzi, P., & Piazza, A. (1994). The History and Geography of Human
Genes. Princeton, NJ: Princeton.University Press.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2001). Sexually Transmitted Disease Surveillance,
2000. Atlanta, GA: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control
and Prevention.

Ellis, L., & Nyborg, H. (1992). Racial/ethnic variations in male testosterone levels: A probable
contributor to group differences in health. Steroids, 57, 72-75.

Entine, J. (2000). Taboo: Why Black Athletes Dominate Sports and Why We Are Afraid to Talk
About It. New York: Public Affairs Press.

Herman-Giddens, M. E., and others. (1997). Secondary sexual characteristics and menses in young
girls seen in the office practice. Pediatrics, 99, 505-512.

Ho, K. C., Roessmann, U., Straumfjord, J. V., & Monroe, G. (1980). Analysis of brain weight.
Archives of Pathology and Laboratory Medicine, 104, 635-645.

Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g Factor. Westport, CT: Praeger.

Lewis, B. (1990). Race and Slavery in the Middle East. New York: Oxford University Press.

Lynn, R., & Vanhanen, T. (2002). IQ and the Wealth of Nations. Westport, CT: Praeger.

Rushton, J. P. (1992). Cranial capacity related to sex, rank, and race in a stratified random

sample of 6,325 U.S. military personnel. Intelligence, 16, 401-413.
Rushton, J. P. (1997). Brain size and cognitive ability in Asian Americans from birth to age seven.
Intelligence, 25, 7-20.

Rushton, J. P. (2000). Race, Evolution, and Behavior: A Life History Perspective (3rd edition).
Port Huron, MI: Charles Darwin Research Institute.

Rushton, J. P., & Ankney, C. D. (1996). Brain size and cognitive ability: Correlations with age,
sex, social class and race. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 3, 21-36.

Rushton, J. P., & Osborne, R. T. (1995). Genetic and environmental contributions to cranial
capacity estimated in Black and White adolescents. Intelligence, 20, 1-13.

Rushton, J. P., & Skuy, M. (2000). Performance on Raven’s Matrices by African and White
university students in South Africa. Intelligence, 28, 251-265.

Stringer, C. & McKie, R. (1996). African Exodus. London: Cape.

Taylor, J., & Whitney, G. (1999). Crime and racial profiling by U.S. police: Is there an empirical
basis? Journal of Social, Political, and Economic Studies, 24, 485-510.

UNAIDS/WHO (2001). AIDS epidemic update: December 2001. Geneva, Switzerland: Joint
United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS (UNAIDS) and World Health Organization
(WHO).

Weinberg, R. A., Scarr, S., & Waldman, I. D. (1992). The Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study:
follow-up of IQ test performance at adolescence. Intelligence, 16, 117-135.

0 comments: